Sign Up for a Free Account
  • Updated 07.21.2024
  • Released 08.06.2001
  • Expires For CME 07.21.2027

Executive dysfunction

Introduction

Overview

The term "executive function" broadly refers to the voluntary regulation of subsidiary cognitive functions that aim for a personally relevant goal, including memory, language, and movements that require skill. Executive function includes maintaining the level of arousal needed for specific tasks, filtering attention, goal-directed behavior, scheduling and prioritizing planned actions, anticipation of the consequences of one's actions, and mental flexibility. The analysis of executive functions represents one of the most important research areas in contemporary neuroscience owing to its necessity for living independently. Historically, executive function has been associated with frontal lobe activity, though subsequent research has indicated that other brain regions also participate. Executive disorders have been documented in a diversity of conditions. Executive deficits early in dementia predict other behavioral disturbances, functional decline, and mortality. In elders, intellectual tasks and everyday activity programs may benefit executive function abilities.

Key points

• Executive functions govern subsidiary cognitive operations, which include problem solving, planning, inhibiting responses, strategy development and implementation, cognitive control, and working memory. Executive functions regulate other cognitions, including language, memory, perception, emotion, and action execution.

• Three major neuroanatomical variants of executive dysfunction syndrome in reference to the frontal lobe can be distinguished: orbitofrontal, medial, and dorsolateral.

• Executive dysfunction has been documented in diverse conditions, including dementia, traumatic brain injury, nonspecific cerebral white matter lesions, borderline personality disorder, substance abuse, multiple system atrophy, multiple sclerosis, schizophrenia, autism, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, progressive supranuclear palsy, CADASIL, and Korsakoff syndrome. It also normally appears in early childhood and generally healthy aging.

• The advent of the worldwide COVID-19 pandemic in 2020 was accompanied by a large number of chronic neurologic disturbances. Among them is executive dysfunction, associated with the common complaint of "brain fog” (impaired focusing of attention). The widespread executive dysfunction that follows COVID illness portends a considerable imposition on public health care resources.

Historical note and terminology

In early research on executive function, based on clinical lesion studies, the frontal lobes were observed to be selectively involved in the self-regulation of voluntary activities, such as problem solving, planning, inhibiting responses, strategy development and implementation, and working memory. Oppenheim, in the 1890s, associated personality changes with the orbital and mesial frontal lobes (142; 143). Luria distinguished three functional units in the brain: (1) arousal-motivation (limbic and reticular systems); (2) receiving, processing, and storing information (postrolandic cortical areas); and (3) programming, controlling, and verifying activity (frontal lobes) (119). Luria mentioned that this third unit has an executive role. "Executive function" as a term has been addressed by many but was coalesced by Lezak to discriminate other cognitions from the "how" or "whether" of human behaviors (111). Lezak emphasized the fluidity of executive function and how the other cognitions and emotion depended on the hypothetical "executive." Baddeley grouped these behaviors into cognitive domains that included problems in planning, organizing behaviors, disinhibition, perseveration, reduced fluency, and initiation (10). Baddeley coined the term "dysexecutive syndrome." Each component of executive function has added to the array of cognitive processes, which include maintaining a problem-solving set for goal-directed behavior, interference control, flexibility, strategic planning, and the ability to anticipate and engage in goal-directed activity (46).

The definition of executive function encompasses actions fueled by conceptualizations, such as the ability to filter interference, engage in goal-directed behaviors, anticipate the consequences of one's actions, and the adaptive concept of mental flexibility (118; 119; 47; 74; 185).

Phineas Gage became a metaphor for frontal lobe dysfunction, and the dysexecutive syndrome became synonymous with frontal lobe pathology. Harlow described Phineas Gage as a responsible foreman for a railroad company who suffered a tragic accident in which a tamping rod explosively rammed through his frontal lobes when he was supervising the construction of the Rutland and Burlington Railroad across Vermont (89). He was reported to have died in a drunken brawl. Initial analyses of Phineas Gage’s skull findings suggested that the injury involved both frontal lobes (43). Computer-generated 3-D reconstructions of a thin-slice computed tomography scan of the trajectory of the rod showed that the brain damage was limited to the left frontal lobe; furthermore, the ventricles and the vital intracranial vascular structures were not affected (158). It was of interest to Harlow that other cognitive functions (ie, memory, language, and sensory motor functions) remained intact, whereas personality was so greatly altered. In this instructional historical case, the “central executive” that regulates other neurologic functions was disrupted but not the basic other, foregoing neurologic functions.

During the late 19th and early 20th centuries, clinical investigations documented diverse behavioral disorders in frontal disease. In 1880, Herman Oppenheim coined the term Witzelsucht, which was demonstrated by childishness and joking with "alleged" cheerfulness (142; 143). The term moria (reflecting "stupidity" and jocular attitude) was part of the change they observed. Oppenheim’s patients all had tumors involving right frontal areas, frequently invading the mesial and basal areas. Jastrowitz noted unconcern and "inappropriate cheerfulness" (100). "Frontal lobe syndrome" was conceptualized by Feuchtwanger (62). He correlated frontal pathology with behaviors that were not related to overt speech, memory, or sensorimotor deficits. He emphasized the personality changes in motivation, affective dysregulation, and the capacity to regulate and integrate other behaviors. Kurt Goldstein expanded the capacity of frontal lobe behaviors to include "the abstract attitude," initiation, mental flexibility, and gaining both the components and the gestalt of the complex environmental arena (78). Goldstein was also sensitive to the compensatory reactions of brain injured individuals coupled with premorbid personality characteristics.

Initially, it was not apparent that "frontal lobe" and "prefrontal cortex" were not synonymous with loci associated with executive dysfunction. The first three decades of the 20th century described precisely the structure of the prefrontal regions. Vincent was one of the first researchers to become aware that the connections to the prefrontal cortex were important to function (199). Early studies elucidated hypothalamic prefrontal connections (200), thus, beginning the research into autonomic and emotional responses of the mesial-orbital prefrontal cortex. It was here that bilateral mesial prefrontal damage that involved the supplementary motor area and the singular cortex was found to produce amotivational akinetic apraxia and motor planning deficits.

When World War II yielded focal brain deficits, frontal lobe pathology was extensively evaluated. Luria related prefrontal lobe activity with programming movement, inhibiting immediate responses as needed, abstracting, problem solving, verbal regulation of behavior, reorienting behavior according to the behavioral consequences, temporal integration of behavior, personality integrity, and consciousness (118; 119).

Currently, executive function research uses functional brain imaging techniques to pool collateral findings, look at antecedents, and use a large sample size to eliminate spurious variables; thus, brain regions that contribute to dysexecutive syndromes may prove to be more multifunctional (116). Functional imaging has demonstrated that adults and children with focal, especially frontal, right-hemispheric lesions display similar behaviors such as attentional deficits, inability to inhibit a response, and impersistence of activity (63).

The relationship between executive functions and so-called general intelligence is not well-defined yet (67). Barbey and colleagues evaluated impairments on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale and Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System in 182 patients with focal brain damage in relation to voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping (13). Abnormal performance in these tests was observed following damage to a distributed network of left lateralized brain areas (frontal and parietal cortex and white matter association tracts). It has also been pointed out that some executive function tests, such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and Verbal Fluency, are closely linked to fluid intelligence. Departing from the hypothesis that fluid intelligence is related to executive function, a confirmatory factor analysis on a mixed neuropsychiatric and nonclinical sample found a high correlation between fluid intelligence and executive functioning (0.91), with working memory being the most profound indicator (196). A moderate to high correlation between crystallized intelligence and executive function was also present. The authors concluded that this study clearly supported the strong association between executive function and intelligence, particularly fluid intelligence. Executive dysfunction observed in some clinical conditions such as Parkinson disease can be interpreted to reflect a decrease in fluid intelligence (160).

This is an article preview.
Start a Free Account
to access the full version.

  • Nearly 3,000 illustrations, including video clips of neurologic disorders.

  • Every article is reviewed by our esteemed Editorial Board for accuracy and currency.

  • Full spectrum of neurology in 1,200 comprehensive articles.

  • Listen to MedLink on the go with Audio versions of each article.

Questions or Comment?

MedLink®, LLC

3525 Del Mar Heights Rd, Ste 304
San Diego, CA 92130-2122

Toll Free (U.S. + Canada): 800-452-2400

US Number: +1-619-640-4660

Support: service@medlink.com

Editor: editor@medlink.com

ISSN: 2831-9125