Neuro-Oncology
NF2-related schwannomatosis
Dec. 13, 2024
MedLink®, LLC
3525 Del Mar Heights Rd, Ste 304
San Diego, CA 92130-2122
Toll Free (U.S. + Canada): 800-452-2400
US Number: +1-619-640-4660
Support: service@medlink.com
Editor: editor@medlink.com
ISSN: 2831-9125
Toll Free (U.S. + Canada): 800-452-2400
US Number: +1-619-640-4660
Support: service@medlink.com
Editor: editor@medlink.com
ISSN: 2831-9125
Worddefinition
At vero eos et accusamus et iusto odio dignissimos ducimus qui blanditiis praesentium voluptatum deleniti atque corrupti quos dolores et quas.
Visual agnosia is the inability to recognize an entity based on its visual features alone despite adequate visual acuity. There are general visual agnosias and selective visual agnosias. General visual agnosias encompass form agnosia, integrative agnosia, and transformative agnosia. Selective agnosias include prosopagnosia (agnosia for faces), pure alexia (agnosia for written words), and topographagnosia (agnosia for locale). General and selective agnosias may be subdivided into apperceptive and associative types. Apperceptive agnosias are characterized by a lack of adequate visual perception of the basic features of an entity to allow for recognition. Associative agnosias are characterized by adequate visual perception but an impairment in associating the visual perception with stored knowledge and meaning of the entity. This article reviews the clinical characteristics, pathogenesis, localization, differential diagnosis, diagnostic evaluation, and rehabilitation of the visual agnosias.
• Visual agnosias comprise a rare group of impairments characterized by an inability to recognize objects by sight despite the preservation of normal visual acuity and the ability to recognize the objects by sound or touch. | |
• The two main visual processing pathways that extend from the primary visual cortex include the dorsal (“where or action”) stream and the ventral (“what”) stream. Visual agnosia occurs because of a problem of the ventral stream. | |
• Visual agnosias are classified into two broad categories: general and selective. Further divisions in both categories include apperceptive visual agnosias (the inability to adequately perceive the basic visual properties of an entity) and associative agnosias (the failure to associate adequate perception with stored knowledge and meaning). | |
• Rehabilitation programs for agnosia remain primarily experimental. |
Descriptions of visual agnosia have existed for over a century. One of the earliest experimental observations involved a dog with partial ablations of both occipital lobes which had lost its usual responses to familiar objects but could still navigate around them, suggesting that it could see but could not recognize (82). In 1890, Lissauer described this “mind-blindness” in humans (72). He distinguished between an apperceptive disorder, in which subjects did not perceive objects well, and an associative disorder, in which subjects could not link what they saw with their stored knowledge about objects (93). In 1891, Freud introduced the term "agnosia," which was later defined more specifically as a failure to recognize objects that could not be attributed to poor visual acuity, cognitive impairment, or aphasia. Despite these early descriptions, controversy regarding whether agnosia was a true entity continued until single-unit recordings and case studies emerged in the 1960s and 1970s (46).
Visual agnosias are divided into two broad categories: general and selective. General and selective agnosias can be further subdivided into apperceptive and associative types. Agnosias can also be divided into those that are developmental and those that are acquired.
The distinction between apperceptive and associative agnosia emerged from descriptions by Lissauer in 1890 (72); he introduced terms that have remained valid ever since. However, the understanding of visual perception and visual recognition has grown considerably since 1890, and discoveries have allowed for arguments suggesting that not all patients can be neatly categorized into apperceptive or associative agnosia (05).
Apperceptive visual agnosia is defined as the inability to visually perceive basic or elemental visual features of an object that results from errors at early visual processing stages. Patients with apperceptive visual agnosia cannot draw or copy viewed objects.
Associative visual agnosia is defined as an inability to visually recognize despite appropriate perception. In associative visual agnosia, recognition fails because it cannot be associated with past knowledge or meaning. Thus, patients with associative agnosia have trouble linking what they see with what they know (62). The stored knowledge may relate to what objects look like (their structural representations) or general semantic facts about objects (their function and where they are found). Patients with associative visual agnosia can often produce a faithful drawing or copy of a viewed object.
Over the years, additional subtypes of each type of visual agnosia have been described, and a more refined taxonomy has emerged (62; 44).
Form agnosia. Patients with this type of apperceptive agnosia have difficulty recognizing even basic shapes, which are the building blocks of object structure (20). However, they can identify certain visual properties, such as brightness, contrast, and motion.
Integrative agnosia. In this type of agnosia, which is usually classified as apperceptive, the integration of shapes into the more complex structures of real objects is impaired (62). For example, a patient might see two round wheels and two triangles but not realize they form a bicycle. This deficit can be shown in their struggles to identify the individual objects within an overlapping figure drawing (62; 56).
Transformation agnosia. Transformation agnosia is a rare apperceptive condition in which patients cannot recognize objects shown from unusual (“non-canonical”) viewpoints. One interpretation is that this deficit reflects a problem with deriving a viewpoint-independent perception of an object’s three-dimensional structure (109; 62).
Category-specific agnosia. In this type of associative agnosia, patients’ recognition of living things like plants and animals might be more deficient than recognition of man-made objects like tools, furniture, or vehicles (45; 70; 24). This category specificity may be explained by the greater visual complexity and similarity of natural objects (63).
Prosopagnosia. In this type of visual agnosia, patients cannot identify familiar faces (07; 35). The deficit is not always confined to faces, extending sometimes to difficulty identifying other objects, such as cars, bicycles, or animals (53; 08). There are apperceptive, amnestic, and associative variants of prosopagnosia (33). In the apperceptive variant, patients cannot see differences in the facial structures of different people. In the amnestic variant, patients cannot recall particular faces. In this amnestic variant, the deficit can be probed with a questionnaire inquiring what patients remember about famous faces (09). In the associative variant, patients have good facial feature perception and preserved facial memories but cannot identify or match a face to a prior memory. There are developmental and acquired forms of prosopagnosia (99).
Pure alexia or word blindness. Pure alexia is a selective associative agnosia also known as “alexia without agraphia.” At the severe end are patients with global alexia who cannot read words at all (21), a deficit that may extend to letters and numbers and other forms of visual symbolic communication, such as musical notation or map symbols (61; 18). At the milder end of pure alexia are patients who can read letters but not words. They recognize words only by reading “letter-by-letter,” reading slowly and taking even longer when word length increases (13).
Topographagnosia. In this selective associative visual agnosia, patients cannot recognize locales or environmental surroundings. A special form includes landmark agnosia, which is the inability to recognize landmarks, such as buildings and scenes (100). Although most topographagnosia is acquired, there is a developmental form (64).
• Patients with visual agnosia may have vague complaints, such as “not able to see properly,” or very specific complaints, such as being “unable to recognize faces.” They typically have insight into the visual nature of their dysfunction by observing that their ability to recognize people depends on nonfacial cues. | |
• Most patients with visual agnosia will attribute the deficit to poor vision and seek help from an optometrist or ophthalmologist. | |
• Patients with visual agnosias can identify objects and entities with tactile or auditory information. |
Diagnosing visual agnosia requires diligence and time, as well as the use of directed questions and specialized testing to discover that a patient’s visual struggles are due to impaired recognition. Many patients will not have insight into the fact that their visual problems are due to brain dysfunction and assume that their perceptual issues stem from an ocular disorder. Those with developmental forms of agnosia may have had the deficit forever and seek the attention of a caregiver only when they realize that other people see things that they are not seeing (84).
Patients with general visual agnosias often report being unable to focus on viewed objects. They mishandle most daily tasks, being unable to recognize coins or paper currency and not knowing which buttons to press on their telephones. They typically attribute their deficit to their eyes and repeatedly consult providers for replacement spectacles. Many will be labeled as having psychogenic disorders.
Prosopagnosia. Patients with the apperceptive variant of prosopagnosia may complain that all faces look similar, like pebbles on a beach. When presented with a sequence of faces spanning various ages, they will have difficulty discerning the youngest and the oldest faces. Patients with the associative variant can discern facial features that enable the detection of age and differences between faces, but if shown a series of famous or familiar faces, they will be unable to recognize them. In either form of prosopagnosia, patients will base recognition on other visual cues, such as hairstyle, distinctive facial features, gait, mannerisms, and voice.
Pure alexia. Patients with pure alexia report that they read slowly or that reading is so laborious or error-prone that they have given it up. They may even report that they cannot read their own writing. Print size and distance to printed material do not influence the reading deficit.
Topographagnosia. Patients with agnosia for locales will get lost in familiar surroundings, including their neighborhood or even their own home. Some patients can compensate by relying on devices with a global positioning system.
The prognosis of agnosia depends on its cause. Patients with small strokes may partially recover. Those with carbon monoxide poisoning, herpes encephalitis, or post-temporal lobectomy often have persistent deficits. Unrelenting slow deterioration is the rule with neurodegenerative diseases that lead to dementia, including those with the posterior form (visual variant) of Alzheimer disease.
Developmental agnosias are stable throughout life, although some have fluctuating psychosocial consequences. For instance, those with developmental prosopagnosia might suffer with the impression that they are aloof and unsociable (31) when starting a new job or interacting with a new group of acquaintances.
Pure alexia. A 41-year-old mechanical engineer who owned an automotive repair shop awoke with numbness in the right face and leg 1 week after an airplane trip. MRI showed a left medial occipital and fusiform gyrus stroke, attributed to paradoxical emboli through a patent foramen ovale. His memory of events was poor initially but quickly returned to normal. He had experienced trouble naming objects, but this quickly normalized, as did the numbness. He reported persistent symptoms of trouble seeing to the right and problems reading. On examination, he had a right homonymous hemianopia. Reading was very slow, proceeding one letter at a time. He could identify single letters and write well. His partner had researched rehabilitative reading strategies, and he had tried using scrolling text and manually tracing letters. He considered that improvement had been modest.
He knew which faces were familiar to him but initially had trouble recalling their names or identities. Formal neuropsychological testing showed difficulties with verbal memory but normal performance on the visual object and space perception battery. On the Warrington Recognition Memory Test, he scored 37/50 for words and 44/50 for faces, and he had normal car recognition. Measures of his reading of single words showed a markedly elevated word-length effect of 1.5 seconds per letter. He was diagnosed with alexia without agraphia (pure alexia) and right homonymous hemianopia.
Experimental studies showed that his problem with visual processing of language impacted written words and the ability to lip-read from faces even though his ability to recognize the identity of faces was intact (94; 03).
The processes associated with visual perception and recognition involve cerebral networks with simultaneous parallel and sequential processing. After visual information reaches the striate cortex in the occipital lobe, information proceeds to extrastriate areas V2 and V3. Thereafter, visual processing is distributed to various cortical regions by means of two main streams (106).
The first pathway is the dorsal (occipitoparietal, “where”) stream, which is dedicated to spatial processing for action and temporal aspects of vision (78). The second pathway is the ventral (occipitotemporal, “what”) stream, which is dedicated to visual recognition. Both pathways have an intermediate level of processing. For the dorsal stream, intermediate areas include V5 and V5a (or V5/MT complex), which are involved in motion processing (114). For the ventral stream, intermediate areas include V4 and V4a, which are involved in color analysis (115).
Identification of any object, visual attribute, or entity involves a network of regions (16) that can be demonstrated using functional magnetic resonance imaging and lesion analysis. As information moves from the retina to cortical networks, more complex representations of the visual information are extracted and associated with nonvisual sensory perceptions, memories, emotions, and behaviors, including limb and body motor responses.
General visual agnosias.
Form agnosia. Form agnosia is associated with widespread bilateral occipital lesions, particularly damage to medial structures, such as the lingual gyri (66). Deficits are especially likely to occur with widespread but nonfocal damage, as occurs after carbon monoxide poisoning, mercury poisoning, or anoxia (02; 17; 71; 23; 79).
Integrative agnosia. Integrative agnosia occurs following bilateral lesions of the lateral and inferior occipital cortex (67), most commonly from peri-striate infarcts or in the visual variant of Alzheimer disease (62; 56). Damage to the right cerebral hemisphere alone can also cause a milder version with prolonged reaction times for naming objects in line drawings (51).
Transformation agnosia. Knowledge of what objects look like from different viewpoints may be stored in both hemispheres, perhaps with a greater “part-based” representation of features in the left hemisphere and more of a “whole-object” representation in the right hemisphere (45; 68).
Category-specific agnosia. Lesions that cause category-specific associative visual agnosia may be related to anterior occipital and posterior temporal damage (88), possibly bilaterally. This type of agnosia may occur when left-sided lesions disconnect posterior occipital areas from more anterior temporal areas (25) or involve the left parahippocampal, fusiform, and lingual gyri (48).
The selective visual agnosias.
Prosopagnosia. Right occipitotemporal or bilateral occipitotemporal lesions can result in prosopagnosia. There are rare cases with left-sided lesions, but in these instances, some subjects were left-handed and may have had atypical hemispheric dominance (105; 76; 06), and others had subtle right-sided anomalies and perhaps should have been considered as having bilateral damage (113).
A network of face areas has been demonstrated by functional MRI (59; 54), including the occipital face area, the fusiform face area, and the superior temporal sulcus, and there is evidence for an anterior inferotemporal area (69; 104). Beyond this core network, an extended network, including the precuneus and inferior frontal gyrus, is sometimes activated by faces (59).
Lesions that cause the apperceptive variant of prosopagnosia are in the inferior occipitotemporal cortex, with loss of the occipital face area, fusiform face area, or both, on the right or both sides (12; 92; 35). Lesions that cause the amnestic variant localize to the right or bilateral anterior temporal cortex. Some patients have both occipitotemporal and anterior temporal damage. In contrast, a lesion of the superior temporal sulcus may impair facial expression perception but leave face identification relatively intact (49).
There is not yet a consensus about the structural basis of developmental prosopagnosia. These subjects do not have gross structural lesions on clinical scans. Some investigators have found decreased fusiform face area activation (57), but others have not (58; 04). Some investigators have reported decreased cortical thickness of the fusiform gyrus (50). Others have argued more for reduced connectivity or white matter damage either in the vicinity of the fusiform gyrus or in the inferior longitudinal fasciculus, which may carry connections between the occipitotemporal face areas and those in the anterior temporal cortex (55; 96; 97; 74; 116; 117).
Pure alexia. Pure alexia has been explained by two hypotheses. The first hypothesis is that it is a disconnection of visual information from the dominant language hemisphere (39; 52). Damage to the left occipital cortex and to callosal fibers in the splenium would cause a homonymous hemianopia and disconnect the left language regions from visual information coming from the intact right occipital lobe. The second hypothesis is that cortical damage to the left fusiform gyrus causes pure alexia (111; 19). In that hypothesis, there is no need to invoke a callosal disconnection, and the left cerebral hemispheric lesion could spare the visual pathway. Indeed, cases of pure alexia without homonymous hemianopia have been reported.
Topographagnosia. In right-handed people, the bilateral parahippocampal gyri and parieto-occipital junctions are involved in processing scene perception. In functional MRI studies, the region activated during scene perception is known as the parahippocampal place area (43). Dysfunction of this region likely explains the association of right occipitotemporal damage with topographagnosia (26).
Although the true incidence of visual agnosias is not well documented, they are known to be rare. Their presumed rarity may reflect the necessity for bilateral lesions in most cases, coupled with the fact that lesions must preserve the retinocortical visual pathway. In some cases, these disorders are not identified by patients or caregivers.
Pure alexia is likely the most often identified type of agnosia because it requires only a unilateral left occipitotemporal lesion. Prosopagnosia is another type of agnosia that can occur after a unilateral lesion, but such a lesion typically causes only a temporary deficit (36; 35).
Developmental agnosias are believed to be more common than acquired disorders. Developmental prosopagnosia reportedly has a prevalence of 2% to 3% (22), an estimate that relies on face recognition test scores falling two standard deviations below a group mean (11).
Vision loss. Vision loss due to ocular or retro-ocular visual pathway disorders is an important cause of an inability to recognize by sight. Although homonymous visual field loss can accompany several selective visual agnosias, the recognition deficit should not be attributed to the field loss. A macular-splitting homonymous hemianopia can reduce reading speed if it involves the central 5 degrees, but such a deficit should be called “hemianopic dyslexia” rather than pure alexia (118; 103). Hemianopic dyslexia has a modest word-length effect of, at most, 120 ms per letter. By contrast, the word-length effect may reach 1 second or more per letter in pure alexia (94). As a precaution, however, patients with impaired reading should not be diagnosed with a recognition disorder until perimetry with special attention to the central 10 degrees of the visual field has excluded small homonymous paracentral scotomas typically caused by infarction of the occipital pole.
Anomia. Naming deficits due to language dysfunction can be mistaken for visual agnosias. Patients with anomia should be able to describe what they see and how to use the object and show its use with gestures. But they cannot name it.
The classic “ventral visual syndrome” is the association of prosopagnosia with a right or bilateral superior homonymous quadrantanopia (77; 32), central dyschromatopsia (the inability to distinguish hues) (81), and topographagnosia (28). This combination of signs is caused by right or bilateral inferior occipitotemporal lesions. When the lesions are bilateral, pure alexia may be present as well.
Prosopagnosia can also be associated with phonagnosia, which is an impaired recognition of familiar voices, and amusia, also known as tone deafness (73; 30).
In developmental prosopagnosia, the patient may fall within the autism spectrum, and face processing can be impaired (107; 10; 65; 101).
Basic visual examination. The examination begins by testing visual acuity and visual fields to ensure that the patient has adequate basic visual functions to allow for visual recognition. If there is impaired recognition of letters or shapes, patients might be unable to identify symbols on the Snellen visual acuity chart. The examiner must then rely on testing visual acuity with the letter E displayed in various orientations (“tumbling E”). The patient indicates the E’s orientation by positioning the fingers of one hand.
The visual field must also be tested formally to rule out loss of the central 5 to 15 degrees, which could impair the identification of objects, faces, or letters.
Cognitive examination. The Mini-Mental State Examination and Montreal Cognitive Assessment aid in screening for cognitive deficits. However, one often needs a formal neuropsychological assessment to identify language deficits or loss of semantic knowledge that does not rely on vision. A thorough evaluation of memory functions can also be completed in a formal neuropsychological assessment, which is particularly important when assessing whether associative visual agnosia is present.
Assessment of general visual agnosias. A diagnostic evaluation for the presence of general visual agnosia can begin by showing patients various items in the room and asking them to name the items, describe what they are, or mime how they are used. Identifying objects in line drawings may be more sensitive because they provide fewer visual cues than real objects. Difficulties on these tests should be compared to the ability to identify the same objects by hearing or touch. If the identification is impaired using nonvisual presentations, the patient has a multimodal problem rather than isolated visual agnosia.
To evaluate for the presence of form agnosia, a patient should be asked to identify simple shapes like squares, triangles, and circles. If they cannot identify these items, a form of agnosia may be present (42; 80). To differentiate a loss of semantic knowledge that does not depend on vision, a patient can be asked about the differences between shapes (“what is the difference between a square and a circle?”). Some patients with form agnosia can identify objects in drawings if they can trace their outlines, translating a visual percept into a kinesthetic one (01; 71).
If patients can identify simple shapes, one can use an overlapping figures test, a robust method of detecting integrative agnosia (62; 56). Patients with this type of visual agnosia also have trouble recognizing the impossibility of images like Escher drawings. Doing so requires an appreciation that the local elements do not integrate into a correct three-dimensional structure (40).
To test for knowledge of object structure, patients may be asked to describe what specific objects look like or to draw them from memory. Their visual access to semantic stores can be probed by showing them several objects and asking them which ones are related and which are not. For example, patients can be asked, “Which is the odd one out?” while viewing pictures of a hammer, wrench, and eggbeater. To ensure that the problem is specific to vision, this performance should be compared to the ability to do the same task with the names of objects rather than pictures.
Other neuropsychological batteries can supplement these bedside tests to distinguish between associative and apperceptive visual agnosias. The Visual Object and Space Perception Battery (VOSP) assesses various elements of object recognition and visuospatial perception (110). In contrast, the Birmingham Object Recognition Battery (BORB) assesses low-level, intermediate, and high-level processing in object recognition (91). Other useful tests include the Cortical Vision Screening Test (CORVIST) (87; 86) and the Leuven Perceptual Organization Screening Test (L-POST) (102).
Assessment of selective visual agnosias. Most of the visual processing necessary for object perception is relatively intact in patients with selective visual agnosias. One or more of the above tests should be completed before assessing for the selective agnosias.
Prosopagnosia. Assessment in the clinic can begin by using photographic albums available on smartphones to assess for familiarity. If the telephone of a family member or spouse is used, the correctness of the patient’s responses can be confirmed.
The Famous Faces test is a useful tool, bearing in mind the patient’s age and culture. Alternatively, short-term familiarity with faces can be assessed by showing several faces in a learning phase, presenting those faces along with others in a recall phase, and asking the patient to indicate which face in the larger set is recalled from the initial set. Formal tests include the Cambridge Face Memory Test (41) and the Warrington Recognition Memory Test (108). With the latter, poor performance on the face half of the test can be contrasted with better performance on the word half. The diagnosis of prosopagnosia is based on a lack of facial recognition but intact voice recognition.
Pure alexia. Pure alexia can be assessed by having the patient read single words and paragraphs in a magazine or book and displaying slowed reading of longer-length words. (13). If patients cannot read words, they can be assessed for reading of numbers and single letters to determine the severity of the deficit (98).
Patients with pure alexia can write much better than they can read. However, some patients have “surface dysgraphia,” an impaired ability to spell and write words with irregular spelling (yacht, colonel). Such a finding denotes a deficit in accessing an internal lexicon (95). Patients with both reading and writing impairment have “alexia with agraphia,” a language domain deficit rather than a visual perceptual deficit. In that circumstance, speech and auditory comprehension should be tested to ensure that poor reading is not simply part of an aphasia. Even without aphasia, reading and writing deficits may be part of a dementia.
Topographagnosia. An initial assessment includes the patient’s ability to recognize familiar landmarks, scenes, or buildings. A collection of pictures from a local city or from famous landmarks (the Eiffel Tower, Niagara Falls) can be used. The photographic library on a family member’s smartphone may serve.
• Strategic adaptations to help patients with impaired object recognition in daily life involve a greater emphasis on tactile or auditory cues or the use of visual cues that are not affected by their disorder. | |
• Various types of computer software can substitute for the deficient skills, but adaptive technology has not reached its full potential. |
Occupational therapists can help patients learn strategies to compensate in part for their deficits. However, there are very few studies to guide the approach. Mobile devices with recognition software have been steadily improving in accuracy and speed, but most patients find these applications and devices cumbersome.
General visual agnosia. Patients with general visual agnosia should be encouraged to use tactile and auditory cues to help recognize objects. The value of putting verbal labels on items in the environment depends on whether reading is spared. Organizing the environment to maintain the same location for items used frequently can reduce the need to search for items using touch. This approach will also allow a person to predict which objects they will encounter in a given place, making recognition easier. Patients should be advised not to drive and counseled about safety concerns in operating heavy machinery.
Prosopagnosia. Patients with prosopagnosia often benefit from learning which individuals will be present in a room before they enter. Patients can also be instructed to depend on voice recognition or on companions aware of their deficits. Patients should be encouraged to explain their deficits to friends and acquaintances when they are comfortable doing so. Nametags can be helpful in the right setting.
Topographagnosia. The widespread availability of devices with GPS capability has made route-finding much easier for patients with topographagnosia, although navigating inside buildings can be difficult. Advanced planning and excursions to familiar locations to discover compensatory methods are also helpful.
Alexia. Substitutive strategies include audiobooks and text-to-speech software applications available on many electronic devices or within audible text pages.
There is no clinical consensus on effective rehabilitative treatments or strategies for visual agnosias. The efficacy of restorative approaches is limited, and a recent systematic review highlights the paucity of rehabilitative investigations and their less-than-impressive results (60).
There is a long history of case reports describing attempts to improve face recognition in prosopagnosia (14; 38; 34). More recently, studies involving small cohorts of patients with prosopagnosia have shown that perceptual learning can lead to modest improvements in face-shape perception (37; 34; 29). Only anecdotal evidence supports the notion that this finding translates into benefits in daily life. One trial found benefits related to oxytocin treatment (14).
None of the treatments used for pure alexia has been accepted as a standard of care. These include increasing the spacing between words or phrases (15; 75), oral articulation during reading (27), repetitive oral reading of the text (15), and finger tracing of letters (75; 83).
There is sparse information about therapies for topographagnosia. Over-learning of familiar routes may help the navigation of some patients in daily life, although this ability may not generalize to new situations.
In the appropriate clinical context, assistive technology has considerable potential to support patients with visual agnosias, particularly those with stable deficits and minimal cognitive impairment. There have been few advances in the practical use of technologies for visual agnosia during the past decade (112). The use of assistive technologies for those with vision loss due to ocular disease is closer to becoming an everyday reality with recent progress in the field (89). This progress gives hope that what is gained and learned using assistive devices for ocular visual impairment can one day be translated into research and development of devices for visual brain dysfunction.
All contributors' financial relationships have been reviewed and mitigated to ensure that this and every other article is free from commercial bias.
Victoria S Pelak MD
Dr. Pelak of the University of Colorado School of Medicine has no relevant financial relationships to disclose.
See ProfileJonathan D Trobe MD
Dr. Trobe of the University of Michigan has no relevant financial relationships to disclose.
See ProfileNearly 3,000 illustrations, including video clips of neurologic disorders.
Every article is reviewed by our esteemed Editorial Board for accuracy and currency.
Full spectrum of neurology in 1,200 comprehensive articles.
Listen to MedLink on the go with Audio versions of each article.
MedLink®, LLC
3525 Del Mar Heights Rd, Ste 304
San Diego, CA 92130-2122
Toll Free (U.S. + Canada): 800-452-2400
US Number: +1-619-640-4660
Support: service@medlink.com
Editor: editor@medlink.com
ISSN: 2831-9125
Neuro-Oncology
Dec. 13, 2024
Neuromuscular Disorders
Dec. 09, 2024
General Neurology
Dec. 09, 2024
Neuro-Oncology
Dec. 05, 2024
Neuro-Ophthalmology & Neuro-Otology
Dec. 02, 2024
Neuro-Ophthalmology & Neuro-Otology
Nov. 24, 2024
Neuro-Ophthalmology & Neuro-Otology
Nov. 22, 2024
Neuro-Ophthalmology & Neuro-Otology
Nov. 22, 2024